Andrew G., Harlow H., Performance of macaque monkey on a test of the concept of generalized triangularity, . Psychol. Monogr., 1948, 19, 3.

., , . I, ., 1907.

ndig A., Latent learning in a water maze, J. exp. Psychoid 1952, 43, 134-137.

evan W., Zener ., Some influences of past experience upon the perceptual thresholds of visual forms, Amer. J. Psychol., 1952, 65, 434-442.

Bierens de Haan J., Werkzeuggebrauch und Werkzeugherstellung bei einem niederen Affen, Zeit. Vergl. Physiol., 1931, 13, 640-695.

- Versuche iiber die Verwendung der Kiste als Schemel bei einigen Procyoniden (Wasch- und Nasenbaren) nebst einigen Bemerkungen iiber das concrete Verstandnis der Tiere im allgemeinen, Zeit. Psychol., 1934, 131, 193-216.

Birch H., Bitterman M., Sensory integration and cognitive theory, Psychol. Rev., 1951, 58, 355-361.

ldgett H., The effect of the introduction of reward upon the maze performance of rats, Univ. Calif. Publ. Psychol., 1929, 4, 113-134.

- Mc Cutchan Mathews R., Spatial learning in the T-maze. The influence of direction, turn and food location, J. exp. Psychol., 1949, 39, 800-809.

Borchgrave Ch. de, L'apprentissage inconscient dans la decouverte d'un principe, Louvain, memoire de licence, 1957.

ral ., The influence of past experience in visual perception, J. exp. Psychol., 1933, 16, 613-643.

Bresson F., Perception et indices perceptifs, : Logique et perception. Etudes d'epistemologie genetique, vol. VII, Paris, Presses Universitaires de France, 1958, 156-185.

ritt J., Tonal sensitivity in the white rat, J. . Psychol., 1935, 19, 243-263.

rgden W., Sensory pre-conditioning, J. exp. Psychol., 1939, 25, 323-332.

- Test of sensory pre-conditioning with human subjects, J. exp. Psychol., 1942, 31, 505-517.

- Sensory pre-conditioning with human subjects, J. exp. Psychol., 1947, 37, 527-539.

Bruce R., Low F., The effect of practice with brief exposure techniques upon central and peripheral visual acuity and a search for a brief test of peripheral acuity, J. exp. Psychol., 1951, 41, 275-280.

Brunswik E., Organismic achievement and environmental probability, Psychol. Rev., 1943, 50, 255-272.

Bugelski ., The psychology of learning, in-8 de 523 p., New York, Holt Cy, 1956.

Bush R., Mosteller F., Stochastic models for learning, in-8 de 365 p., New York, Wiley, 1955.

Buytendyk F., Fischel W., Versuch einer neuen Analyse der tieriechen Einsicht, Arch. Neerland. Physiol., 1931, 16, 449-476.

Caldwell W., Jones H., Some positive results on a modified Tolman and Honzik insight maze, J. . physiol. Psychol., 1954, 47, 416-418.

asn H., The conditioned pupillary reaction, J. exp. Psychol., 1922, 5, 108-146.

- Trowbridge M., An experimental study of Thorndike's theory of learning, J. gen. Psychol., 1932, 7, 245-260.

Cook ., Factors in massed and distributed practice, J. exp. Psychol., 1944, 34, 325-334.

rsland H., Taylor H., Newsn S., Practice and improvability in the Muller-Lyer illusion in relation to intelligence, J. gen. Psychol., 1929, 2, 290-306.

Culler E., Nature of the learning curve, Psychol. Bull., 1928, 25, 143-144.

Dashiell J., Direction-orientation in maze learning by the white rat, m. Psychol. Monogr., 1930, 7, 1-72.

De Lann J. D., La perception de la forme elle-meme, Journ. Psychol. Norm. Path., 1962, 59, 59-74.

Djang S., The role of past experience in the visual apprehension of masked forms, J. Exp. Psychol., 1942, 30, 1-22.

Dre L., Hilgard E., Spaced practice and the maturation hypothesis, J. Psychol., 1937, 4, 245-259.

Drever J., The pre-insight period in learning, Brit. J. Psychol., 1934, 25, 197-203.

Elliott M., Some determining factors in maze performance, Amer. J. Psychol., 1930, 2, 315-317.

Estes W., Toward a statistical theory of learning, Psychol. Rev., 1950, 57, 94-107.

- and others, Modern learning theory, in-8 de 379 p., New York, Appleton Century, 1954.

- The statistical approach to learning theory, Koch, Psychology. A Study of a Science, vol. 11, New York, McGraw Hill, 1959, 380-491.

Faltheim A., Learning, problem-solving and after-effects, Uppsala, 1956, 195 p.

Fields P., Studies in concept formation: 1. The development of the concept of triangularity by the white rat, J. m. Psychol., 1931, 11, 326-366.

Finger F., The effect of varying conditions of reinforcement upon a simple running response, J. exp. Psychol., 1942, 30, 53-68.

Galanter E., Place and response learning: learning to alternate, J. m. physiol. Psychol., 1955, 48, 17-18.

Gavini H., Apprentissage et transfert dans le seuil differentiel de luminance, Bull. C.E.R.P., 1962, 11, 13-24.

Gellermann L., Form discrimination in chimpanzee and two year old children, J. genet. Psychol., 1933, 42, 3-27.

Gibson E., Improvement in perceptual judgments as a function of controlled practice, Psychol. Bull., 1953, 50, 401-431.

Gibson J., Gibson E., Perceptual learning: differenciation or enrichment, Psychol. Rev., 1955, 62, 32-41.

Gleitman H., Place learning without prior performance, J. m. physiol. Psychol., 1955, 48, 77-79.

Gottschaldt K., Uber den Einfluss der Erfahnung auf die Wahrnehmung von Figuren, Psychol. Forsch., 1926, 8, 247-260.

Grant D., The influence of attitude on the conditioned eyelid response, J. exp. Psychol., 1939, 25, 33-346.

Guillaume P., Limitation chez l'enfant, in-8 de 221 p., Paris, Alcan, 1925.

- Meyerson I., Recherches sur l'usage de l'instrument chez les singes:

1. Le probleme du detour, J. Psychol, norm, path., 1930, 27, 177-236;

2. L'intermediaire lie a l'objet, ibid., 1931, 28, 481-555;

3. L'interme-diaire independant de l'objet, ibid., 1934, 31, 499-554;

4. Choix, correction, invention, ibid., 1937, 34, 425-448.

- La formation des habitudes, in-8 de 166 p., Paris, Alcan, 1936.

Guthrie E., Conditioning as a principle of learning, Psychol. Rev., 1930, 37, 412-428.

- The psychology of learning, in-8 de 258 p., New York, Harper, 1935.

- Association by contiguity, Koch, Psychology. A Study of a Science, New York, McGraw Hill, 1959, 158-195.

Heidbreder E., An experimental study of thinking, Arch. Psychol., 1924, 73.

- The attainment of concepts: 4. The process, J. Psychol, 1947, 24, 93-138.

- The attainment of concepts: 6. Exploratory experiments on conceptualization at perceptual levels, J. Psychol., 1948, 26, 193-216.

Henl ., An experimental investigation of past experience as determinant of visual form perception, J. exp. Psychol., 1942, 30, 1.22.

Hilgard E., Conditioned eyelid reactions to a light stimulus based on the reflex wink to sound, Psychol. Monogr., 1931, 41, 1-50.

- Campbell A., The course of acquisition and retention of conditioned eyelid responses in man, exp. Psychol., 1936, 19, 227-247.

- Humphreys L., The effect of supporting an antagonistic voluntary instruction on conditioned discrimination, J. Exp. Psychol., 1938, 22, 291-304.

- Marquis D., Conditioning and learning, in-8 de 429 p., New York, Appleton Century, 1940.

- , : c , , . II, ., 1963, 11-65.

- Theories of learning, in-8, New York, Appleton Century, Ire ed. 1948, 409 p., 2e ed., 1956, 563 p.

Hill C., Thune L., Place and response learning in the white rat under simplified and mutually isolated conditions, J. exp. Psychol., 1952, 43, 289-297.

Hnzik C., Central control in the maze learning of rats, J. m. Psychol., 1933, 15, 95-132.

Hvland C., Experimental studies in rote learning theory: 6. Comparison of retention following learning to same criterion by massed and distributed practice, J. exp. Psychol., 1940, 26, 568-587.

, : c ., , . II, ., 1963, 124-223.

Hsiao H., Experimental study of the rat's insight within a spatial complex, Univ. Calif. Publ. Psychol., 1919, 4, 57-70.

Hudgins C., Conditioning and voluntary control of the pupillary light reflex, J. gen. Psychol., 1933, 8, 351.

Hull C, Quantitative aspects of the evolution of concepts, Psychol. Monogr., 1920, 123.

- Knowledge and purpose as habit mechanisms, Psychol. Rev., 1930, 37, 511-525.

- Priciples of Behavior, in-8 de 422 p., New York, Appleton Century, 1943.

- Essentials of Behavior, in-8 de 144 p., New Hawen, Yale Univ. Press, 1951.

- A Behavior system, in-8 de 372 p., New Hawen, Yale Univ. Press, 1952.

Humphrey G., The Nature of learning, in-8 de 296 p. London, Kegan P., 1933.

Humphreys L., The effect of random alternation of reinforcement on the acquisition and extinction of conditioned eyelid reactions, J. exp. Psychol., 1939, 25, 141-158.

- Extinction of conditioned psychogalvanic responses following two conditions of reinforcement, J. exp. Psychol., 1940, 27, 71-75.

- Measures of strength of conditioned eyelid responses, J. gen. Psychol., 1943, 29, 101-111.

Humes J., The effect of practice upon the upper limit for tonal discrimination, Amer. J. Psychol., 1930, 42, 1-16.

Hunter W., Learning: 4. Experimental Studies of learning, : Handbook of General Experimental Psychology, Worcester, Clark Univ. Press, 1934, 497-570.

Jenkins J., Sheffield F., Rehearsal and guessing habits as sources of the "spread" of effect., J. exp. Psychol., 1946, 36, 316-330.

Jenkins W., Stanley J., Partial reinforcement: a review and critique, Psychol. Bull., 1950, 47, 193-234.

Jones H., Conditioned psychogalvanic responses in infants, Psychol. Bull., 1928, 25, 183-184.

Jonckheere A., Modeles stochastiques et apprentissage, : Logique, apprentissage et probabilite. Etudes d'epistemologie genetique, vol. VIII, Paris, Presses Universitaire de France, 1959, p. 139-172.

Johnson E., The role of motivational strength in latent learning, J. m. physiol. Psychol., 1952, 45, 526-530.

Jst A., Die Assoziationsfestigkeit in ihrer Abhangigkeit von der Verteilung der Wiederholungen, Zeitschr. Psychol., 1897, 14, 436-472.

arn H., Sensory preconditioning and incidental learning in human, J. exp. Psychol., 1947, 37, 540-544.

ientzle M., Properties of learning curves under varied distributions of practice, J. exp. Psychol., 1946, 36, 187-211.

ilpatrik F., Two processes in perceptual learning, J. exp. Psychol., 1954, 47, 362-370.

Kimble G., Hilgard and Marquis' Conditioning and learning, 2e ed. New York, Appleton Century, 1961.

luver H., Behavior mechanisms in monkeys, in-8 de 387 p., Univ. Chicago Press, 1932... Kohler I., Uber Aufbau und Wandlungen der Wahrnehmungsveld, in-8 de 118 p., Wien, Roher, 1951.

Kohler W., Intelligenz Priifungen an Menschenaffen, Berlin, Springer, 1917.

- Fischback J., The destruction of the Muller-Lyer illusion in repeated trials, J. exp. Psychol., 1950, 40, 267-281, 398-410.

nrski J., Miller S., Nouvelles recherches sur les reflexes conditionnels moteurs, CR. Soc. Biol., 1934, 115, 91-96.

- Miller S., On two types of conditioned reflexes, J. gen. Psychol., 1937, 16, 264-267.

- Conditioned reflexes and neuron organization, in-8 de 267 p., Cambridge Univ. Press, 1948.

- Mechanisms in learning, in Symp. Soc. Exp. Biol., 4, Cambridge Univ. Press, 1950, 409-431.

., Die letzten Forschritte in der Methodik der Erforschung der Bedingten Reflexe in Kindern, Jahrb. f. Kindern, 1926, 114, 256-269.

Lashle ., The human salivary reflex and its use in psychology, Psychol. Rev., 1916, 23, 446-464.

- Brain mechanisms and intelligence, in-8 de 186 p., Chicago Univ. Press, 1929.

- Ball J., Spinal conduction and kinaesthetic sensitivity in the maze habit, J. m. Psychol., 1929, 9, 71-106.

- Learning. Nervous mechanisms in learning, : Foundations of Experimental Psychology, Worcester, Clark Univ. Press, 1929, p. 456-496.

Le N J. - Fr., Le conditionnement, Paris, Presses Universitaires de France, 1961.

Lewis D., Partial reinforcement: a selective review since 1950, Psychol. Rev., 1960, 57, 1-28.

Lrge I., Influence of regulary interpolated time intervals upon subsequent learning, Teach. Coll. Contr. Educ, 1930, n. 438.

Luhins A., Social influences on perception of complex drawings, J. soc. Psychol., 1945, 22, 279-296.

Lumsdaine A., Conditioned eyelid responses as mediating generalized conditioned finger reactions, Psychol. Bull., 1939, 36, 650.

Lyon D., The relation of length of material to time taken for learning and the optimum distribution of time, J. educ. Psychol., 1914, 5, 1-9, 85-91, 155-163.

Mac Corquodale K., Meehl P., Preliminary suggestions as to a formalization of expectancy theory, Psychol. Rev., 1953, 60, 55-63.

Mac Farlane D., The role of kinaesthesis in maze learning, Univ., Calif. Public. Psychol., 1930, 4, 277-305.

Maier N.R.F., Reasoning in white rats, . Psychol. Monogr., 1929, 6, 1-93.

Marx M., Spread of effect. A critical review, Genet. Psychol. Monogr., 1956, 53, 119-186.

M Fadden H., Three studies in psychological optics, Duncam, Okla, Optometric Extension Program, 1941, 1-26.

M Geh J., The psychology of human learning, in-8 de 633 p., New York, Longmans Green, 1942.

Mc Neill H., Motor adaptation and accuracy, in-8 de 303 p., Louvain, edit. Inst. Sup. Phil., 1934.

Meehl P., Mac Corquodale R., A further study of latent learning in the T-maze, J. . physiol. Psychol., 1948, 41, 372-396.

Menzies R., Conditioned vaso-motor responses in human subjects, J. Psychol., 1937, 4, 75-120.

. ., Les infusoires peuventils apprendre a choisir leur nourriture?, Arch. f. Protistenk, 1914, 34, 60-78.

Michotte A., Reflexions sur le role du langage dans l'analyse des organisations perceptives, Actes Congr. intern. Psychol., Bruxelles, 1957.

Miller S., Konorski J., Sur une forme particuliere de reflexe conditional, CR. Soc. Biol., 1928, 99, 1155-1157.

Miller J., The effect of facilitory and inhibitory attitudes on eyelid conditioning, Psychol. Bull., 1939, 36, 577-578.

Miller G., Dollard J., Social learning and imitation, in-8 de 341 p., New Haven, Yale Univ. Press, 1941.

Mobius K., Die Bewegungen der Tiere und ihre psychischer Horizont, Sohr. Naturw, Ver. Schl., 1873, 1, 113-130.

Mntpellier G. de, Les alterations morphologiques des mouvements rapides, in-8 de 251 p., Louvain, Inst. Sup. Phil., 1935.

- Colle J., Reactions conditionnees volontaires et involontaires, Arch. psychol., 1939, 27, 134-156.

- Conduites intelligentes et psychisme chez l'animale et chez l'homme, in-8, de 414 p., Louvain, Ed, Inst. Sup. Phil., 1946 et 1949.

- Conditionnement et apprentissage, : Le conditionnement et apprentissage, Symposium Assoc, psychol. scientifique de Langue franchise, Paris, Presses Universitaires de France, 1958, 107-151.

- Theories de l'apprentissage et conditions de la performance, Annie psychol., 1959, 59, 107-116.

Murad Y., L'eveil de l'intelligence, in-8 de 406 p., Paris, Alcan, 1943.

Mwrer O., Jones H., Habit strength as a function of pattern of reinforcement, J. exp. Psychol., 1945, 35, 293-311.

- Learning theory and personality dinamics., in-8 de 776 p., New York, Ronald Press, 1950.

- Two-factor learning theory: Summary and comment, Psychol. Rev., 1951, 58, 350-354.

- Two-factor theory reconsidered with special reference to secondary reinforcement and the concept of habit, Psychol. Rev., 1956, 63, 114-128.

- Learning theory and Behavior, in-8 de 555 p., New York, Wiley, 1960.

Mukherjee K.., The duration of cutaneous sensation and the improvement of its sensible discrimination by practice, J. exp. Psychol., 1933, 16, 339- 342.

Muller G., Pilzeker A., Experimentelle Beitrage zur Lehre vom Gedachtnis, Zeitsch. Psychol., 1900, 1, 1-288.

Nelting G., La structuration perceptive de la figure de Muller-Lyer en fonction de la repetition chez enfant et chez l'adulte, Arch, de Psychol., 1960, 37, 311-413.

Norton F., Kenshalo D., Incidental learning under condition of unrewarded irrelevant motivation, J. m. physiol. Psychol., 1954, 47, 375-377.

Nuttin J., De wet van effekt en de rol van de Taak in het leerprocess, Louvain, Diss. Doct., 1941.

- Tache reussite et echec, in-8 de 530 p., Louvain, Publ. Univ., 1953.

Oakes W., An experimental study of pitch naming and pitch discrimination reactions, J. gen. Psychol., 1955, 86, 237-259.

Osgood Ch., Method and theory in experimental psychology, in-8 de 800 p., New York, Oxford Univ. Press, 1953.

Osgood Ch., Motivational dynamics of language behavior, Nebrasks Symposium on Motivation, in-8 de 311 p., Lincoln, Nebraska Univ. Press, 1956.

Parreren C, Psychologie van het leren, in-16 de 251 p. Arnhem, W. de Haan, 1960.

., . . ., . III, . IV, . - ., 1951.

Phaub M., Caldwell W., Perceptual learning: differenciation and enrichement of past experience, J. gen. Psychol., 1959, 60, 137-157.

Piaget J., Apprentissage et connaissance, : Etudes d'epistemologie genetique, vol. VII, p. 21-67; vol. X, p. 159-188, Paris, Presses Universitaires de France, 1959.

Piern H., L'evolution de la memoire, in-16 de 136 p., Paris, Flammarion, 1910.

- L'habitude et la memoire, : Nouveau Traite de Phychologie, vol. IV, Paris, Alcan, 1934, 67-136.

- Recherches experimentales sur les phenomenes de memoire, Annee psychol., 1913, 19, 91-193.

- Vocabulaire de psychologie, in-8 de 356 p., Paris, Presses Universitaires de France, 1951. H., Observations sur l'excitabilite des infusoires, Russ. Arch. Protist, 1928, 7, 1-24.

Pollet R. Le probleme de l'inconsient dans l'apprentissage, Louvain, Diss. Doct., 1946.

., ., Des modes d'associations cerebrales chez l'hommeetchez les animaux, J. Psychol, norm, path., 1926, 23, 1020-1028.

Razran G.H.S., Conditioned withdrawal responses with shock as the conditioned stimulus in adult human subjects, Psychol. Bui., 1934, 3, 143.

- Conditioned responses: an experimental study and a theoretical analysis, Arch. Psychol., 1935, 191, 1-124.

- Attitudinal control of human conditioning, J. Psychol., 1936, 2, 327-337.

Renshaw G., The visual perception and a reproduction of forms in tachistoscopic methods, J. Psychol., 1945, 20, 217-232.

Re A., Choix adapte precedant la prise de conscience, Arch. Psychol., 1935, 25, 157-178.

Reynolds ., A repetition of the Blodgett experiment on "latent learning", J. exp. Psychol., 1945, 35, 504-516.

Robinson E. W. A., A preliminary experiment on abstraction in a monkey, J. m. Psychol., 1933, 16, 231-236.

Rozeboom W., What is learned? An empirical enigma, Psychol. Rev., 1958, 65, 22-33.

Saltzman E., The orienting task in incidental and intentional learning, Amer. J. Psychol., 1953, 66, 593-597.

Seashore R., Stanford motor skills unit., Psychol. Monogr., 1928, 39, 51-66.

- Work methods: an often neglected factor underlying individual differences, Psychol. Rev., 1939, 46, 123-141.

Seward J., The effect of practice on the visual perception of form, Arch. Psychol., 1931, 20, 130.

Sheffield F., Temmer H., Relative resistance to extinction of escape training and avoidance training, J. exp. Psychol., 1950, 40, 287-298.

Shipley W., Conditioning the human plantar reflex, J. exp. Psychol., 1932, 15, 422-426.

- An apparent transfer of conditioning, J. gen. Psychol., 1933, 8, 382-391.

- Indirect conditioning, J. gen. Psychol., 1935, 12, 337-357.

Skinner ., Two types of conditioned reflex and a pseudo-type, J. gen. Psychol., 1935, 12, 66-77.

- Two types of conditioned reflex: a reply to Konorski and Miller, J. gen. Psychol., 1937, 16, 272-279.

- The behavior of organisms, in-8 de 457 p. New York, Appleton Century, 1938.

Smith F., The effect of training in pitch discrimination, Psychol. Monogr., 1914, 16, 69.

Smoke K., An objective study of concept formation, Psychol. Monogr., 1932, 49, 191.

Solomon R., Wynne L., Traumatic avoidance learning: the princip anxiety conservation and partial irreversibility, Psychol. Rev., 1954, 61, 353-386.

Spaet I., Harlow H., Solution by rhesus monkeys of multiple sign-problems utilizying the oddity-technique, J. . Psychol., 1943, 35, 119-132.

c ., , : , , . II, ., 1963, 224-273.

- Behavior theory and conditioning, in-8 de 262 p. New Hawen, Yale Univ. Press, 1956.

- Behavior theory and learning, in-8 de 403 p., New York, Prentice Hall, 1960.

Stevenson H., Latent learning in children, J. Exp. Psychol., 1954, 47, 17-26.

Stratton G., Vision without inversion of the retinal image, Psychol. Rev., 1897, 4, 341-360, 463-481.

Switzer S., Disinhibition of the galvanic skin response, J. gen. Psychol., 1933, 9, 77-100.

Szentkereszty M., L'experience de l'"espece unique" chez les enfants de 8 a 12 ans, Louvain, memoire de licence, 1956.

Thistlethwaite D., A critical review of latent learning and related experiment, Psychol. Bull., 1951, 48, 97-129; 1952, 49, 61-71.

- Condition of irrelevant incentive learning, J. m. physiol. Psychol., 1952, 45, 517-525.

Thorndike E., Educational Psychology: 2. The psychology of learning, in-8 de 452 p., New York, Teachers' College, 1913.

- Human learning, in-8 de 206 p., New York, Century, 1931.

- Fundamentals of learning, in-8 de 638 p., New York, Teachers' College, Columbia, 1932.

- An experimental study of rewards, in-8 de 72 p., New York, Teachers' College, Columbia, 1933.

- A theory of action of the after-effect of a connection upon it, Psychol. Rev., 1933, 40, 435-439.

- The psychology of wants, interests and attitudes, in-8 de 301 p., New York, Century, 1935.

Thorpe W., Learning and instinct in animals, in-8 de 493 p., London, Methuen, 1956.

Thurstone L. L. A., The learning curve equation, Psychol. Monogr., 1919, 114.

- The learning function, J. gen. Psychol., 1930, 3, 469-493.

Tolman E., Bretnall E., H a 1 1 C, A disproof of the law of effect and a substitution of the laws of emphasis, motivation and disruption, J. exp. Psychol., 1932, 15, 601-614.

- Hnzik C., "Insight" in rats, Univ. Calif. Public. Psychol., 1930, 4, 215-232.

- Hnzik C., Introduction and removal of reward and maze performance of rats, Univ. Calif. Public. Psychol., 1930, 4, 257-275.

- Purposive behavior in animals and men, in-8 de 463 p., New York, Century, 1932.

- Operational behaviorism and current trends in psychology, Proc. 25th Ann. Univ. South. Calif., 1936, 89-103.

- Theories in learning, : Moss, Comparative Psychology, New York, Prentice Hall, 1934, 367-408.

- Ritchie ., alish D., Studies in spatial learning: 1. Orientation and the short-cut, J. exp. Psychol., 1946, 36, 13-24; 2. Place learning versus response learning, J. exp. Psychol., 1946, 36, 285-292.

- Cognitive maps in rats and men, Psychol. Rev., 1948, 55, 189-208.

- Gleitman H., Studies in spatial learning: 7. Place and response learning under different degress of motivation, J. exp. Psychol., 1949, 39, 653-659.

- There is more than one kind of learning, Psychol. Rev., 1949, 56, 144-155.

- Principles of performances, Psychol. Rev., 1955, 62, 315-326.

- Principles of purposive behavior, .: Koch, Psychology. A Study of a Science, vol. , New York, McGraw Hill, 1959, 92-157.

Thompson M., Thompson J., Reactive inhibition as a factor in maze learning: 2. The role of reactive inhibition in studies of place learning: versus respons learning, J. exp. Psychol., 1949, 39, 883-895.

Travis R., Lenght of the fraction period and efficiency in motor learning, J. exp. Psychol., 1939, 24, 339-345.

Triplett N., The educability of the perch., Amer. J. Psychol., 1901, 12, 354-360.

Van der Veldt J., L'apprentissage du mouvement et l'automatisme, in-8 de 350 p., Louvain Inst. Sup. Phil., 1928.

Walker E., Drive specificity and learning: demonstration of a response tendency acquired under a strong irrelevant drive, J. m. physiol. Psychol., 1952, 44, 596-603.

Wallh H., Henle M., An experimental analysis of the law of effect, J. exp. Psychol., 1941, 28, 340-349.

- O'Cnnell D., Neisser U., The memory effect of visual perception of the threedimensional form, J. exp. Psychol., 1953, 45, 360-368.

Warner L., An experimental search for the "conditioned response", J. genet. Psychol., 1932, 41, 91-115.

Watson J., Behavior: An introduction to comparative psychology, in-8 de 439 p., New York, Holt, 1914.

- Psychology from the standpoint of a behaviorist, in-8 de 458 p., Philadelphia, Lippincott, 1919.

Webb W., A study in place and response learning as a discrimination behavior, J. m. Psychol., 1951, 44, 263-268.

Wendt S., An analytical study of the conditioned knee-jerk, Arch. Psychol., 1930, 18, 1-97.

Wever E., The upper limit of hearing in cat, J. m. Psychol., 1930, 10, 221-234.

Williams K., The reward values of a conditioned stimulus, Univ. Calif. Public. Psychol., 1929, 4, 31-55.

Wilcox W., An interpretation of the relation between visual acuity and light intensity, J. gen. Psychol., 1936, 15, 405-435.

Wipple G., Studies in pitch discrimination, Amer. J. Psychol., 1903, 14, 289-304.

Wolfe H., On the estimation of the middle of lines, Amer. J. Psychol., 1923, 34, 313-358.

- Time factors in conditioning finger-winthdrawal, J. gen. Psychol., 1930, 3, 372-378.

- Conditioning as a function of the interval between the conditioned and the original stimulus, J. gen. Psychol., 1932, 7, 80-103.

Wdrw H., The relation between abilities and improvement with practice, J. educ. Psychol., 1938, 29, 215-230.

Woodworth R., Schlosberg H., Experimental Psychology, in-8 de 948 p., New York, Holt Cy, 1954.

Woodworth R., Dynamics of behavior, in-8 de 403 p., New York, Holt Cy, 1958.

Watt R., Improvability of pitch discrimination, Psychol. Monogr., 1945, 58, 267.

Zener ., The significance of behavior accompanying conditioned salivary secretion for theories of the conditioned response, Atner. J. Psychol., 1937, 50, 385-403.

Zirk1e G., Success and failure in serial learning: 1. The Thorndike effect; 2. Isolation and the Thorndike effect, J. exp. Psychol., 1946, 36, 230-236, 302-315.

Yerkes R., Methods of exhibiting reactive tendencies characteristic of ontogenic and phylogenic stages, J. Amln. Behav., 1917, 7, 11-28. - The mind of a gorilla, Genet. Psychol. Monogr., 1927, 2, 1-193, 375-551; m. Psychol. Monogr., 1928, 5, 1-92.









.., .., 2001-2019
:
http://psychologylib.ru/ ' '

@Mail.ru